Women in the U.S. who live in homes surrounded by more vegetation appear to have significantly lower mortality rates than those who live in areas with less vegetation, according to a new study from Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health and Brigham and Women's Hospital. The study found that women who lived in the greenest surroundings had a 12% lower overall mortality rate than those living in homes in the least green areas.
The study suggests several mechanisms that might be at play in the link between greenness and mortality . Improved mental health, measured through lower levels of depression, was estimated to explain nearly 30% of the benefit from living around greater vegetation. Increased opportunities for social engagement, higher physical activity, and lower exposure to air pollution may also play an important role, the authors said.
The study will be published online April 14, 2016 in the journal Environmental Health Perspectives .
"We were surprised to observe such strong associations between increased exposure to greenness and lower
mortality rates ," said Peter James, research associate in the Harvard Chan School Department of Epidemiology. "We were even more surprised to find evidence that a large proportion of the benefit from high levels of vegetation seems to be connected with improved mental health."
Previous studies have suggested that exposure to vegetation was related to lower mortality rates, but those studies were limited in scope, and some had contradictory findings. The new study is the first to take a nationwide look at the link between greenness and mortality over a period of several years.
The study incorporated data on 108,630 women enrolled in the Nurses' Health Study across the United States in 2000-2008. The researchers compared the participants' risk of mortality with the level of vegetation surrounding their homes, which was calculated using satellite imagery from different seasons and from different years. The researchers accounted for other mortality risk factors, such as age, socioeconomic status, race and ethnicity, and smoking behaviors.
When the researchers looked at specific causes of death among the study participants, they found that associations between higher amounts of greenness and lower mortality were strongest for respiratory-disease and cancer mortality. Women living in areas with the most vegetation had a 34% lower rate of respiratory disease-related mortality and a 13% lower rate of cancer mortality compared with those with the least vegetation around their homes. These more specific findings were consistent with some of the proposed benefits of greener areas, including that they may buffer air pollution and noise exposures and provide opportunities for physical activity.
"We know that planting vegetation can help the environment by reducing wastewater loads, sequestering carbon, and mitigating the effects of climate change. Our new findings suggest a potential co-benefit—improving health—that presents planners, landscape architects, and policy makers with an actionable tool to grow healthier places," said James.
More information: "Exposure to Greenness and Mortality in a Nationwide Prospective Cohort Study of Women," Peter James, Jaime E. Hart, Rachel F. Banay, Francine Laden, Environmental Health Perspectives , online April 14, 2016, doi: 101289/ehp.1510363
Provided by: Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health
Friday, April 15, 2016
More exposure to vegetation linked with lower mortality rates in women
Tuesday, April 12, 2016
Why body weight loss in Tuberculosis Patient?
Decreased plasma leptin concentrations in tuberculosis patients are associated with wasting and inflammation.
Tuberculosis patients often suffer from severe weight loss, which is considered to be immunosuppressive and a major determinant of severity and outcome of disease. Because leptin is involved in weight regulation and cellular immunity, its possible role in tuberculosis-associated wasting was investigated. In an urban clinic in Indonesia, plasma leptin concentrations, indicators of adipocyte mass, appetite, C-reactive protein (CRP), tuberculin reactivity, and cytokine response were measured in tuberculosis patients and healthy controls. Plasma leptin concentrations were lower in patients than in controls (615 vs. 2,550 ng/liter; P < 0.001). Multivariate regression analysis showed that body fat mass and inflammation were two independent factors determining plasma leptin concentrations; there was a positive correlation between fat and leptin, whereas, unexpectedly, leptin was inversely associated with CRP and tumor necrosis factor-alpha production. Concentrations of both CRP and leptin were independently associated with loss of appetite. Our results do not support the concept that weight loss in tuberculosis is caused by enhanced production of leptin. Rather, loss of body fat leads to low plasma leptin concentrations, and prolonged inflammation may further suppress leptin production. Because leptin is important for cell-mediated immunity, low leptin production during active tuberculosis may contribute to increased disease severity, especially in cachectic patients.
Biomarker discovery offers hope for new TB vaccine
This photomicrograph
reveals Mycobacterium tuberculosis bacteria using acid-fast Ziehl-Neelsen
stain; Magnified 1000 X. The acid-fast stains depend on the ability of
mycobacteria to retain dye when treated with mineral acid or an …more
A team of scientists led
by Oxford University have made a discovery that could improve our chances of
developing an effective vaccine against Tuberculosis.
The researchers have
identified new biomarkers for Tuberculosis (TB) which have shown for the first
time why immunity from the widely used Bacillus Calmette-Guérin (BCG) vaccine
is so variable. The biomarkers will also provide valuable clues to assess
whether potential new vaccines could be effective.
TB remains one of the
world's major killer diseases, causing TB disease in 9.6 million people and 1.5
million deaths in 2014. The only available vaccine, Bacillus Calmette-Guérin
(BCG), works well (estimated 50% effective) to prevent severe disease in
children but is very variable (0% to 80% effective) in how well it protects
against lung disease, particularly in countries where TB is most common.
While BCG is one of the
safest and most widely used vaccines worldwide, there is one key issue: It is
currently very difficult to determine whether it will work or not. This also
makes it really hard to determine if any new vaccines might work.
For many vaccines,
medics and scientists can use what are called immune correlates or biomarkers,
typically in the blood, which can be measured to determine whether a vaccine
has successfully induced immunity. Not only are these correlates useful in
measuring the success of existing vaccination programmes, they are also invaluable
in assessing whether potential new vaccines could be effective.
With a pressing need for
a TB vaccine that is more effective than BCG, a research team drawn from a
number of groups at Oxford University, working with colleagues from the South
African Tuberculosis Vaccine Initiative at the University of Cape Town and the
London School of Hygiene & Tropical Medicine, set out to identify immune
correlates that could facilitate TB vaccine development. The team, funded by
the Wellcome Trust and Aeras, and led by Professor Helen McShane and Dr Helen
Fletcher, studied immune responses in infants in South Africa who were taking
part in a TB vaccine trial.
Professor McShane said:
'We looked at a number of factors that could be used as immune correlates, to
try and find biomarkers that will help us develop a better vaccine.'
The team carried out
tests for twenty-two possible factors. One - levels of activated HLA-DR+CD4+
T-cells - was linked to higher TB disease risk. Meanwhile, BCG-specific
Interferon-gamma secreting T-cells indicated lower TB risk, with higher levels
of these cells directly linked to greater reduction of the risk of TB.
Antibodies to a TB
protein, Ag85A, were also identified as a possible correlate. Higher levels of
Ag85A antibody were associated with lower TB risk. However, the team cautions
that other environmental and disease factors could also cause Ag85A antibody
levels to rise and so there may not be a direct link between the antibody and
TB risk.
Professor McShane said:
'These are useful results which ideally would now be confirmed in further
trials. They show that antigen-specific T cells are important in protection
against TB, but that activated T cells increase the risk".
Dr Helen Fletcher from
the London School of Hygiene & Tropical Medicine, said: "For the first
time we have some evidence of how BCG might work, and also what could block it
from working. Although there is still much work to do, these findings may bring
us a step closer to developing a more effective vaccine for TB."
Dr Tom Scriba from the
South African Tuberculosis Vaccine Initiative said: 'TB is still a major
international killer, and rates of TB disease in some areas of South Africa are
among the highest in the world. These findings provide important clues about
the type of immunity TB vaccines should elicit, and bring us closer to our
vision, a world without TB.'
The team is continuing
its work to develop a TB vaccine,
aiming to protect more people from the disease.

More information: T cell activation is an Immune Correlate
of Risk in BCG vaccine infants, Nature Communications, April 12,
2016, DOI:
10.1038/NCOMMS11290
Monday, April 11, 2016
New findings reveal social thinking in the infant brain
An innovative
collaboration between neuroscientists and developmental psychologists that
investigated how infants' brains process other people's action provides the
first evidence that directly links neural responses from the motor system to
overt social behavior in infants.
The research will be
published April 12 in Psychological Science, the peer-reviewed
journal of the Association for Psychological Science.
The study involved
thirty-six 7-month-old infants,
who were each tested while wearing a cap that used electroencephalography (EEG)
to measure brain activity. During the experiment, each infant observed an actor
reach for one of two toys. Immediately after, the baby was allowed to select
one of the same toys. This procedure was repeated 12 times.
Babies' brain activity
predicted how they would respond to the actor's behavior. When the infants
recruited their motor system while
observing the actor grasp one of the toys, they subsequently imitated the
actor. When they didn't imitate the actor, there was no detectable engagement
of the motor system in their brain activity as they watched the actor.
"Our research
provides initial evidence that motor system recruitment is contingently linked
to infants' social interactive behavior," said lead author Courtney
Filippi, a doctoral candidate in developmental psychology at the University of
Chicago. "It provides initial evidence that recruiting the motor system
during action encoding predicts infants' subsequent social interactive
behavior."
Untested possibility
The researchers used EEG
to measure a component of brain activity—desynchronization of activity in the
mu frequency band—that has been linked to motor cortex activity in adults. Like
adults, infants show this response when acting themselves and when watching
others' actions, suggesting that the motor system may play a role in the perception
of others' actions. Until the current study, however, this possibility had not
been tested in infants.
"This research
tells us that, by the middle of their first year of life, babies are beginning
to be able to understand that people act intentionally—that they choose one toy
over another because they want that toy," said Helen Tager-Flusberg,
professor of psychological and brain sciences at Boston University, who is
familiar with but was not involved in the research. "This understanding on
the part of a baby involves not just seeing the other person's action, but also
involves the baby's own motor system, which is recruited when he or she chooses
the same toy."
Fundamentally, the
researchers identified the neural processes that contribute to intelligent social
behavior in infants. And it's the first evidence that motor system activation
in infants predicts the imitation of others' actions, as well as an apparent
understanding of others' goals.
"This is big news,
that babies understand what they are observing, that there is a direct
connection between observing others, understanding what others are doing, and
learning how to act," said co-author Amanda Woodward, the William S. Gray
Professor of Psychology at UChicago.
The researcher's
methodology also broke new ground. "This is the first attempt to combine
the assessment of infants' behavior—in this case, imitating the actions of
another person—with measuring brain activity in infants," Tager-Flusberg
said.
Proof of concept
"Probably the
hardest place to study the relation between brain activity and behavior is with
infants, due to limitations in the methods that can be used, and the fact that
infants are infants," Woodward noted. "Our methodology represents a
breakthrough and a proof of concept."
"We've worked hard
over the years to develop the methods that allow us to record brain activity from
infants while they are engaged in the social world," said co-author Nathan
Fox, Distinguished University Professor at the University of Maryland, College
Park. "The current research reflects our ability to synchronize brain and
behavior in infants during the first year of life."
Although this research
will not translate directly into new medical treatments or therapies, it could
contribute to medical advances down the road by helping to illuminate how the
human brain functions and develops, Woodward added.
"One reason to
engage in basic science is to better understand the development of the brain
and mind. Here we looked at the development of social cognition, social behavior,
and the motor system,
all of which are critical for human development and are often disrupted in
developmental disabilities, including autism."
New study shows rich, poor have huge mortality gap in US

New study shows rich, poor have huge mortality
gap in US
“As you go up in the income
distribution, life expectancy continues to increase, at every point in the
income distribution,” Michael Stepner says. Credit: Christine Daniloff/MIT
Poverty in the U.S. is often
associated with deprivation, in areas including housing, employment, and
education. Now a study co-authored by two MIT researchers has shown, in
unprecedented geographic detail, another stark reality: Poor people live
shorter lives, too.
More precisely, the study
shows that in the U.S., the richest 1 percent of men lives 14.6 years longer on
average than the poorest 1 percent of men, while among women in those wealth
percentiles, the difference is 10.1 years on average.
This eye-opening gap is also
growing rapidly: Over roughly the last 15 years, life expectancy increased by
2.34 years for men and 2.91 years for women who are among the top 5 percent of
income earners in America, but by just 0.32 and 0.04 years for men and women in
the bottom 5 percent of the income tables.
"When we think about
income inequality in the United States, we think that low-income Americans
can't afford to purchase the same homes, live in the same neighborhoods, and
buy the same goods and services as higher-income Americans," says Michael
Stepner, a PhD candidate in MIT's Department of Economics. "But the fact
that they can on average expect to have 10 or 15 fewer years of life really
demonstrates the level of inequality we've had in the United States."
Stepner and Sarah Abraham,
another PhD candidate in MIT's Department of Economics, are among the
co-authors of a newly published paper summarizing the study's findings, and
have played central roles in a three-year research project establishing the
results.
In addition to reporting the
size and growth of the income gap, the study finds that the average lifespan
varies considerably by region in the U.S. (by as much as 4.5 years), but that
the sources of that regional variation are subtle, and, like the aggregate
national gap, subject to further investigation.
"The patterns are not
exactly what you might expect," says Abraham, noting that regional
variation in longevity does not seem strongly correlated with factors such as
access to health care, environmental issues, income inequality, or the job
market.
"We don't find those to
be as highly correlated with differences in longevity as we find measures of
health behavior, such as smoking rates or obesity rates" [to be correlated
with lifespan], Abraham observes.
The paper, "The Association
between Income and Life Expectancy in the United States, 2001-2014," is
being published today by the Journal of the American Medical Association.
The authors are Raj Chetty, a
professor of economics at Stanford University; Stepner and Abraham of MIT, who
are the second and third authors on the paper; Shelby Lin, an analyst with
McKinsey and Company in New York; Benjamin Scuderi, a predoctorate fellow in
Harvard University's Economics Department; Augustin Bergeron, a PhD candidate
in Harvard University's Economics Department; Nicholas Turner of the Office of
Tax Analysis in the U.S. Department of the Treasury; and David Cutler, a
professor of economics at Harvard University.
The geography of mortality
The researchers looked at 1.4
billion anonymized income tax filings from the federal government, and combined
that with mortality data from the years 2001 through 2014 from the Social
Security Administration. This represents the most complete geographic and
demographic landscape of mortality in America.
Among other things, the
growth of the gap in mortality rates—by nearly three years—struck the
researchers as noteworthy. To put it in perspective, they note that federal
health officials estimate that curing all forms of cancer would add three years
to the average lifespan.
"That change over the
last 15 years is the equivalent of the richest Americans winning the war on
cancer," Stepner observes.
At the same time, the
researchers are quick to point out that the findings cannot immediately be
reduced to simple cause-and-effect explanations. For instance, as social
scientists have long observed, it is very hard to say whether having wealth
leads to better health—or if health, on aggregate, is a prerequisite for
accumulating wealth. Most likely, the two interact in complex ways, something
the study cannot resolve.
"It's a descriptive
story," Stepner says of the data.
A new puzzle emerging from
the study, the authors note, is that differences in lifespan exist along the
entire continuum of wealth in the U.S.; it is not as if, say, the top 10
percent of earners cluster around identical average lifespans.
"As you go up in the
income distribution, life expectancy continues to increase, at every
point," Stepner says.
And then there are the new
geographic patterns in the findings. For instance: Eight of the 10 states with
the lowest life expectancies for people in the bottom income quartile form a
contiguous belt, curving around from Michigan through Ohio, Indiana, Kentucky,
Tennessee, Arkansas, Oklahoma, and Kansas.
So while average lifespans
for everyone are lower in some Southern states, the poor do not fare worse in
those places than they do in other regions.
"The Deep South is the
lowest-income area in America, but when we're looking at life expectancy
conditional on having a low income, it's not worse to be poor in the Deep South
than it is in other areas of America," Stepner says. "It's just that
there are far more poor people living in the South."
Future research: Think local
The researchers say that more
analysis on the sources of local variation in lifespans could be among the most
fruitful research areas stemming from the current paper. The research team is
releasing all the data from the study today as well.
Among the municipalities
where low-income people have experienced the greatest increases in lifespan
from 2001-2014, for example, are Toms River, New Jersey; Birmingham, Alabama;
and Richmond, Virginia. Cities with the largest drops in lifespan among the
poor are Tampa and Pensacola, Florida; and Knoxville, Tennessee.
"We're not making any
normative statements about what policy should be, but there is a wide
dispersion of [results] happening in the U.S.," Abraham says. "That
might need to be addressed at a more granular level."
Places with the overall
longest lifespans for the poor include New York City, with a chart-topping 81.8
years on average, as well as a passel of cities in California. The bottom of
that list includes Gary, Indiana (77.4 years on average); Las Vegas; and
Oklahoma City.
Among the top income earners,
people live longest in Salt Lake City (87.8 years on average); Portland, Maine;
and Spokane, Washington. The rich have the shortest lives in Las Vegas (84.1
years on average); Gary, Indiana; and Honolulu.
Abraham also observes that
the findings could have implications for national policy programs, as well.
"Things like Social
Security aren't going to be as redistributive if the richer people are getting
paid for 10 more years than the poorer people," she says.
Overall, the researchers say
they hope to spark a larger discussion among the research and policy
communities.
"We don't have all the
answers," Abraham says. "But it's really important to make these
statistics widely used so people have an idea of what the magnitude of these
problems is, where they might focus their attention, and why this
matters."
Explore further: New report examines
implications of growing gap in life span by income for entitlement programs
More information: Raj Chetty
et al. The Association Between Income and Life Expectancy in the United States,
2001-2014, JAMA (). DOI: 10.1001/jama.2016.4226
Read more at:
http://phys.org/news/2016-04-rich-poor-huge-mortality-gap.html#jCp
Employment status affects our morals around money
In the study 'Moral consequences of becoming unemployed',
endorsed by the prestigious scientific journal PNAS, researchers at the
UPV/EHU's Faculty of Economics and Business and at the University of Nottingham
(UK) have analysed a moral consequence of unemployment that together with the
effects it has on people's mental health, could explain why these people become
disengaged from the labor market. 151 young adults in Córdoba and Bilbao were
involved in the study.
As Luis Miller asserts, "in general, both people in
employment and those in full-time education believe that people should be
allowed to keep most of what they earn and that it is OK for those who work
harder or who are more productive to earn more". He went on to say,
"When people become unemployed, our study indicates that they let go of
this belief. They put a higher value on the redistribution of money, which, in
social terms, would mean higher taxes on those earning more in order to fund
increased public spending."The authors of the study Luis Miller, lecturer
at the UPV/EHU's Faculty of Economics and Business, and Paloma Ubeda, a UPV/EHU
researcher, highlight the importance of understanding how becoming unemployed
affects people's behaviour. Many studies link unemployment and poverty with
depression, anxiety, stress, low levels of well-being and self-esteem, high
suicide rates, murder, alcohol-related deaths, etc. In this study, however, the
researchers have looked at a different kind of effect and have concluded that
unemployment changes people's morals around the distribution of money. It
should be pointed out that the study has been published in the latest edition
of the PNAS, a prestigious scientific journal which publishes relatively few
papers from the social sciences but which has given particular importance to
this one.
"In our study," explained Paloma Úbeda, "we
didn't ask the participants about re-distribution, taxes or public spending, as
the responses to questions of this type could be biased by the self-interest of
the interviewees. So high earners who look after their own interests would
prefer lower taxes, while low earners who also have their own interests in mind
would want higher taxes. What we were really interested in was understanding
how, when becoming unemployed, people change the way they see what is fair in
terms of re-distribution, in other words, whether they change their moral
values. We found that they do; when becoming unemployed people change the way
they think about fairness and re-distribution".
Unemployment leads to changes in opinions
To research people's ideals about justice, the researchers
involved 151 young adults aged between 18 and 35 in the so-called
"Distributive Justice Game", an experiment designed to reveal the
values and preferences of the participants about fairness and re-distribution.
The experiment was conducted in Bilbao and Córdoba. The game consisted of two
parts.
In the first part the participants "worked" for
the researchers for seven minutes. In the second part of the game each
participant was given a tray divided into four sections. Each section contained
a different amount of money. One of the sections belonged to the participant
who had been given the tray. The other three sections belonged to the other
three participants who were in their playing group. For some groups the amount
of money each one received depended on how much work the people had done during
the first part of the game. In others, the amount of money on the trays
depended totally on luck and were not related in any way to the work each
person had done. The participants could re-distribute the money among the four
sections in whatever way they wanted. Each one could keep all the money, leave
the tray as they had received it or re-distribute the money so that the four
participants would all receive the same amount at the end of the experiment.
"We found that the employed people tended to
re-distribute the money less when they knew people had earned their money in
the first part," asserted Luis Miller. "By contrast, they tended to
re-distribute it almost equally when they knew that the initial distributions
were just due to luck".
The 151 young adults participated twice in the experiment,
the first time in the spring of 2013 and the second exactly one year later.
Repeating the experiment allowed the researchers to see whether the people who
were employed or in full-time education during the first year of the study but
ended up unemployed in the second year changed their opinions about fairness
and re-distribution.
Most of those who had become unemployed re-distributed the
money in such a way that the other three members of their group ended up with
approximately the same amount of money irrespective of whether the money had
been earned or received as a result of luck.
Paloma Úbeda added that "the extent to which people
recognise the individual right to keep what they have earned has significant
implications on the way people vote, on how they pay their taxes or on how they
act in the labour market. However, all these implications would need to be
studied in greater detail in future pieces of research".
Luis Miller concluded that "the significance of the
main result of this study to understand the labour dynamics as well as the most
appropriate public intervention depends to what extent the negative effect we
find can be reversed. Right now, we are already working on new projects that
seek to establish whether the unemployed need to re-acquire part of the values
relating to effort and productivity abandoned along the way before effectively
reengaging with the labour market. Then assuming they do, we would need to
investigate how this new change of values takes place and also how public
interventions can contribute towards enabling this process".
Explore further:
Study results suggest people are less cooperative in unequal societies when
wealth inequality is evident
More information: Moral consequences of becoming unemployed
, PNAS,www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1521250113
Subscribe to:
Posts (Atom)